Journal
Looking Over vs. Overlooking Native American Languages: Let’s Void the Void
Title
Looking Over vs. Overlooking Native American Languages: Let’s Void the Void
Publication Type
Journal Article
Year of Publication
1996
Authors
Stubbs, Brian D. (Primary)
Journal
Journal of Book of Mormon Studies
Pagination
1-49
Volume
5
Issue
1
Abstract
The time-depth of the Romance language family (ca. 2,000 years) yields an abundance of similarities among languages descended from Latin: Spanish, French, Italian, and so forth. The time-depth of Lehi is not much greater (2,600 years), yet no similar abundance of accepted linguistic evidence for Lehi’s presence in the Americas has emerged. Is this because of a lack of evidence or a lack of looking? We cannot know until we look. The relative absence of effort in Native American languages relevant to Book of Mormon research is a huge void in Latter-day Saint scholarly endeavor. This paper discusses the value of and need to void this existing void, and presents from one Native American language family an example of the possibilities.
Looking Over vs. Overlooking Native American Languages:
Let's Void the Void
[For correct symbols and diacritic marks, please see the pdf version of this article. Ed.]
Abstract: The time-depth of the Romance language family (ca. 2,000 years) yields an abundance of similarities among languages descended from Latin: Spanish, French, Italian, and so forth. The time-depth of Lehi is not much greater (2,600 years), yet no similar abundance of accepted linguistic evidence for Lehi’s presence in the Americas has emerged. Is this because of a lack of evidence or a lack of looking? We cannot know until we look. The relative absence of effort in Native American languages relevant to Book of Mormon research is a huge void in Latter-day Saint scholarly endeavor. This paper discusses the value of and need to void this existing void, and presents from one Native American language family an example of the possibilities.
Our traditional approach to language-related research regarding the Book of Mormon has been fairly thorough and productive in traditional directions, but an established imbalance in that approach has left a void in what should be an important sphere of Latter-day Saint research: linguistic analysis of Native American languages. Though the void is understandable for the past—because of limited data and too few scholars—both limits are now changing sufficiently to allow efforts toward voiding this void.
As believers in the Book of Mormon, we adhere to the actuality that parties accompanying Lehi and Mulek left Jerusalem and arrived in the Americas some 2600 years ago and that their descendants are among the Native Americans. The writings of John Sorenson and others suggest that descendants of those immigrant parties and the geographical locations they originally occupied were much less than the pan-American assumptions of earlier generations.1 The immense linguistic variety in the Americas suggests the same. Some 2,000 Native American languages comprise nearly 100 separate language families. That diversity leaves little doubt that many peoples besides the groups of Lehi and Mulek contributed to pre-Colombian populations and languages. Nevertheless, whatever the original parameters of geography and language for the Book of Mormon peoples, it is not unreasonable to expect that evidences of Hebrew or possibly Egyptian may survive in some languages of the Americas.
Thus far the focus of Book of Mormon language research has been Hebrew, Egyptian, and the translated English text. This logical starting place, subject to careful thought and study, has yielded enlightening results; nevertheless, another dimension awaits attention. Though the number of Latter-day Saint scholars knowing Hebrew, Egyptian, or related languages has increased, we hardly suffer from an overabundance of those knowledgeable in ancient Near Eastern languages. In terms of employability or marketability of that knowledge, some individuals may feel part of an overabundance, but in a strict academic sense for collective research purposes, in light of what remains to be done, our resources are still fairly limited, especially if we consider the other sphere of research that remains quite untouched: Native American languages.
What was the language of Mormon and Moroni? Debates among Latter-day Saint scholars center on Hebrew and Egyptian; however, both may be near misnomers for the Lehi languages of A.D. 400. Between Lehi and Moroni was a span of approximately 1,000 years, and between Moroni and European contact was a little more than 1,000 years. Thus Moroni was about midway between Lehi and European contact. The extent that Book of Mormon groups had been in contact with or had mixed with non-Hebrew speakers by Moroni’s time would likely parallel the degree of change in the languages of Lehi’s posterity by A.D. 400. No known Native American language is very similar to Hebrew (or Egyptian). Suppose that the American language(s) most similar to Hebrew were identified and that the amount of change from Hebrew was interpolated over the more than two millennia since Lehi’s arrival. If the Lehi languages of A.D. 400 had undergone about half the lexical and grammatical change observable in the Native American language(s) most similar to Hebrew, that amount of change would leave Moroni’s and the Lamanites’ language(s) of A.D. 400 more significantly different from either Hebrew or Egyptian than most suspect. Old English, largely because of foreign influences over the last 1,000 years, is essentially a foreign language to modern English speakers, though both forms are called “English”; and the language differences between Lehi’s Hebrew and the languages of his posterity 1,000 years later may have exceeded the changes in English in a similar length of time. Therefore, if Native American languages are not much further removed in time from Moroni than was Lehi, maybe the contemporary end of the timeline can provide as many clues as Lehi’s end, if not a greater number and clearer clues. Thus why not investigate both ends of the language spectrum?
In any case, we know practically nothing about the languages in Moroni’s day, but we do know something about the Hebrew and Egyptian of the Old World that Lehi left, and we have hundreds of languages in the New World where he arrived. Why is nearly all of our Book of Mormon-related language research confined to only one of two ends of the language spectrum? The two-language end is certainly easier to deal with than the 2,000-language end, but that cannot be perpetual justification for a body of scholars in search of truth to ignore indefinitely a huge reservoir of research potential—Native American languages. An adjustment now seems desirable. In fact, the present may be an optimum time for some to consider this larger sphere of research, since just now substantive quantities and qualities of data are accumulating for comparative research in many Native American languages.
Since research in this “larger picture” requires a combination not common in scholarly preparation, most interested persons would need to expand their backgrounds. Three prerequisites—a knowledge of Hebrew or other Near Eastern languages, a foundation in historical linguistics, and a knowledge of a Native American language family—qualify one for the work, so to speak. For those who already know Hebrew, adding a background in historical linguistics would allow investigation of a Native American language family with some potential for results. For linguists accomplished in Native American languages, adding Hebrew or related languages to their language repertoire would provide a similar package of prerequisites. Perhaps this oblique invitation might better apply to young prospective scholars still in the stage of preparation than to established scholars already set in research specialities.
Though I want to encourage, I must also, in all fairness, first caution against romanticized expectations of swift results. The realm of research in Native American languages is infinitely fascinating, but for mortals possessing a mere lifetime, infinite fascinations can also be frustrations. Though most scholarly accomplishment requires sizable portions of a lifetime, contrast the required language base for research endeavor in the ancient Near East vs. the Americas. A knowledge of half a dozen languages (Hebrew/Phoenician, Arabic, Aramaic, Egyptian or Coptic, Akkadian, and Greek) provides one with a fairly complete array of ancient Near Eastern languages. Would that six languages could do the same for a specialist in Uto-Aztecan, Hokan, or Penutian (each consisting of ca. 30 languages), or for one interested in proposed relationships between Uto-Aztecan, Penutian, and Kiowa-Tanoan (involving three language families totaling more than 60 languages), or for one like myself interested in a dozen language families, totaling a few hundred languages.
A second caution worth mentioning is that one not assume that Native American languages are less complex or easier to learn than Hebrew, Arabic, or Egyptian. Let me express my own opinion on the matter.2 Though Arabic (but not necessarily Hebrew) may have a richer lexicon than what has been preserved or recorded for most Native American languages, the structural complexities (phonological, morphological, and syntactic) of many, if not most, Native American languages leave Hebrew, Arabic, and Egyptian easier to learn than, for example, Navajo (or any other Athapaskan language), Ute, Cora, most Hokan, most Penutian, or Kiowa-Tanoan languages. On the other hand, not all Native American languages are so complex: for example, Hopi, Tarahumara, Quechua, and Muskoguean languages are no more difficult and probably easier to learn than Arabic or Egyptian.
A third caution not to be overlooked is that Native American language families are, for the most part, linguistically more complex than Semitic. Few language families on earth are so neat, clear-cut, and problem-free as Semitic. Though every language family has unresolved problems (e.g., exceptions to sound correspondences, etc.), such problems apply to perhaps less than 10% of the Semitic lexicon, while 50% of the Uto-Aztecan cognate sets are complicated by departures from the understood sound correspondences (cognates are words in related languages descended from the same word in a former parent language). Hokan and Penutian are still hypotheses, since no one has yet been able to produce a convincing system of sound correspondences for either group. Though most linguists see sufficient similarity within each group to think that they are separate groups of related languages, neither is yet a proven language family. I recently heard Margaret Langdon, the foremost Hokanist for decades, say, “Some days I wonder if Hokan is a fantasy.” As for elusiveness from definitive linguistic analysis, Indo-European is somewhere between Semitic and most Native American language families. One difference is that a virtual army of linguists has contributed solutions to Indo-European over the last century and a half, while Native American language families typically attract perhaps ten to twenty linguists working on individual languages and three or four interested in comparative work on the language family as a whole.
These observations hint at the volume of data and difficulties an Americanist faces; and in an effort to be both an Americanist and a Semitist, which I see as the only total approach to Book of Mormon language matters, one can feel overwhelmed and wonder at the imbalance—that nearly all interested Latter-day Saint scholars seem to focus on the two-language end, while ignoring the equally important 2,000-language end.
In any case, we must be cautious in our expectations of what we might find and in our interpretations of those findings. Even if a connection between Hebrew (or Egyptian) and a Native American language family were established, it would not necessarily prove the Book of Mormon, since a Semitic element, if found, could possibly have arrived independent of Lehi and Mulek. On the other hand, a lack of a connection would not necessarily disprove it either, since lack of a Near East language element could be because of language loss or change among a people, as has happened often in the histories of language groups. For example, Aramaic had replaced Hebrew as the common vernacular among the Jews by Jesus’ time, and the Iberian populations adopted Latin under Roman rule. Yet the language of a conquering people does not always prevail. In the Iberian Peninsula the Germanic Visigoths actually adopted the language of the people they conquered, speaking later forms of Latin. Most Native Americans now speak English or Spanish, though hardly of Indo-European ancestry. Many more examples could be cited. In other words, language and lineage may or may not have much to do with each other.
Nevertheless, a language element traceable to Northwest Semitic found among American languages would only strengthen the plausibility of the sacred record’s historicity in ancient America. Beyond that, if some tribal names or place names were found to match Hebrew forms of Book of Mormon peoples or places, or if written records were discovered and deciphered, and their language found to be something linguistically between Old World Semitic and New World languages, or their deciphered contents were to align with events or peoples mentioned in the Book of Mormon text, then it would be refreshing to have some answers and a new set of questions.
In any case, we are admonished to “study and learn, and become acquainted . . . with languages, tongues, and people” (D&C 90:15), and comparative linguistic research among Native American groups should hold a higher priority among Latter-day Saint scholars than it has, since those efforts can apply or relate to so many interests relevant to Book of Mormon scholarship. Yet it seems fair to say that serious comparative linguistic investigation with respect to the Book of Mormon has been a void in Latter-day Saint endeavor. Not only is it relevant to the other disciplines focusing on the Book of Mormon, but comparative linguistic research may prove to be the very key to answers thus far evading other modes of investigation. It has the potential of giving us the basic vocabulary of certain ancient American groups; relative percentages of Hebrew and Egyptian; possible identification of dialects, ethnic compositions, and places of departure; and more.
Also worth noting is the relative strength of comparative linguistic evidence. The nature of comparative linguistic evidence provides large bodies of data—several thousand words per language—that is nonforgeable. Ruins and buildings yield some facts, though who built them is not always one of the facts revealed. Words of a translation can be debated endlessly, and written records can feasibly be forged, but no one can fabricate a language family of several Native American tribes speaking a variety of related languages.
In spite of the potential, it is important to note that no American Indian language has yet been shown to descend from or relate to a Near Eastern language, at least to the satisfaction of the linguistic community. My research of over a hundred languages and several language families thus far has convinced me that no Native American language so obviously and solely descends from Hebrew or Egyptian in the way that Spanish, French, and Italian so clearly descend from Latin. Nevertheless, even though no pervasive appearance of Hebrew in the Americas has surfaced, hints of Hebrew occur in a number of language families.
Some language families contain more similarities to Hebrew than could be attributed to chance, while other language families tease with enough promising leads to merit further investigation. However, in all such cases, if a Near Eastern linguistic element should prove verifiable, it seems clear that this element has mixed heavily with other languages quite dissimilar to Hebrew or Egyptian, because all Native American languages have many features very different from Hebrew and Egyptian. This accords well with Sorenson’s views of “others in the land.”3 Nonetheless, some languages may contain a Hebrew component. Because of the immensity of American linguistic diversity, the nature of responsible linguistic investigation, and a current severe shortage of those interested and prepared to investigate, progress in rigorously sifting and tracing the leads will necessarily be slow. Nonetheless, an example of the possibilities is in order.
The language family that I have dealt with most is Uto-Aztecan (UA), in which I have identified substantial similarities with Hebrew. A short preview of the growing case for a Hebrew element in UA seems appropriate for students of the Book of Mormon. Let me emphasize the word element, for UA languages are very different from Hebrew in many ways. In other words, in addition to a Hebrew element in UA, any Hebraist learning or reading a UA language can readily see more differences than similarities, supporting the other half of my thesis, that this Hebrew element is mixed heavily with non-Near Eastern elements.
In addition to numerous lexical similarities, some features of Northwest Semitic morphology are still productive in UA, i.e., are still functionally active, such as the masculine plural suffix and niqtal prefix, while much more is fossilized, i.e., nonfunctional “frozen” patterns are detectable such as the feminine plural, qittel forms, hiqtîl and huqtal forms, etc. With that in mind, consider a few of some 1,000 identified similarities between Hebrew and Uto-Aztecan.4
A Hebrew Element in Uto-Aztecan
The UA language family consists of the following languages:
Branch | Language (abbreviation) | Locale |
North UA | ||
Western Numic | Mono (Mn); Northern Paiute (NP) | CA, OR, NV |
Central Numic | Panamint (Pn); Shoshone (Sh);Comanche (Cm) | NV NV, UT, ID, WY TX |
Southern Numic | Kawaiisu (K); Chemehuevi (Ch); Southern Paiute (SP); Ute (U) | S. CA UT, CO |
Takic | Cahuilla (Ca); Luiseño (Ls); Serrano (Sr); Cupeño (Cp); Gabrielino (Gb) | S. CA |
single- language branches | Tubatulabal (Tb) Hopi (Hp) | S. CA AZ |
South UA | ||
Tepiman | O)odham/Papago/Pima (Od) Northern Tepehuan (NT) Southern Tepehuan (ST) | AZ, Mex Mex Mex |
Cahitan | Yaqui (Yq); Mayo (My) | Mex |
Sonoran | Tarahumara (Tr); Guarijio (Wr) Tubar (Tbr); Eudeve (Eu) | Mex Mex |
Corachol | Cora (Cr); Huichol (Hch) | Mex |
Aztecan | Nahuatl (N) | Mex |
For a pronunciation guide to the sounds as represented in this paper, see the appendix, Orthography and Pronunciation (pages 43–45), which I encourage the reader to consult now. Abbreviations other than those listed above are found at the end of the appendix. Sources for lexical items from the various Native American and Semitic languages are listed in the bibliography. A proto-language is a hypothesized parent language from which a group of related languages descended; an asterisk (*) before a form or word signifies that it has been reconstructed by linguists as an unattested ancient or intermediate form in the parent language on the basis of comparisons of related words (cognates) in the descendant languages.
Among the most interesting discoveries are certain similarities of UA forms to archaic voweling patterns in Northwest Semitic, the branch to which Hebrew belongs.
Hebrew | UA | |||
1. | plural suffix | -îm | *-ima | |
2. | passive/rfl/rcp prefix | ni- | *na- | |
3. | perfect of yšb | sit/dwell | yašab | *yasipa |
UA morphemes show some similarity with Masoretic Hebrew, though nothing exact: -îm and -ima; ni- and na-; yašab and yasipa. However, the facts that Hebrew -îm came from an earlier *-ima; the Hebrew niqtal (or nip(al) prefix ni- from an earlier *na-; and Hebrew yašab from an earlier *yašiba, all establish a nearly perfect identity between pre-Hebrew (proto-Northwest Semitic) and proto-UA forms:
NW Sem | UA | |
plural suffix | *-ima | *-ima |
reflexive/reciprocal prefix | *na- | *na- |
sit, dwell | *yašiba | *yasipa5 |
Furthermore, the verbal forms of both Northwest Semitic and UA contain semantic dimensions of *yašiba, which means “sit” and “dwell” in both families. That the UA voweling patterns are quite equivalent to proto-Northwest Semitic voweling patterns is striking. The Hebrew Old Testament text as we have it, also known as the Masoretic text, was voweled by the Masoretes some 1,200 to 1,300 years after Lehi and Mulek left Jerusalem. Thus that form of Hebrew known as biblical Hebrew is only one dialect of ancient Hebrew, and is a very late dialect at that, far removed from Lehi and David. Though the consonants of the text, written much earlier, are more reliable, the voweling patterns of the Masoretic dialect of Hebrew are as far removed in time from Lehi’s Hebrew as U.S. Southern English is from Old English, which two forms of English are also 1,200 years apart and are very different. Hebrew, as we know it, lost the short final vowels of proto-Semitic, but as seen in 1 and 3, those vowels are apparent in UA. However, not all UA forms preserve the phonology so well, for in most cases UA has phonologically reduced Semitic forms greatly; nevertheless, archaic features do turn up sporadically.
It is worth noting that the above items help point to Northwest Semitic (as opposed to other branches of Semitic or Semitic generally) and sometimes, specifically Hebrew, as having the closest affinity to UA.
masculine plural | sit/dwell | |
Arabic (South Sem) | -una/-ina | wataba |
Aramaic | -în | yətib |
Akkadian (East Sem) | -u/-i | ašabu |
Ugaritic | -uma/-ima | )atib |
pre-Heb/NW Semitic | *-ima | *yašiba |
UA | *-ima | *yasipa |
One can see that n and not m appears in the masculine plural suffix in Arabic and Aramaic, while East Semitic lacks both n and m. Only Northwest Semitic shows -ima. Ugaritic belongs to Northwest Semitic as Hebrew does. So these all point to Northwest Semitic for the plural suffix. The forms for “sit/dwell” point even more specifically to Hebrew. Proto-Semitic and South Semitic w corresponds to Hebrew y, and Ugaritic and East Semitic lack either initial w or y, all of which suggests Hebrew. Likewise, Aramaic, Ugaritic, and South Semitic all show t (θ) rather than š; the intersection of these two sets (y and š) points only to Hebrew in the verb “sit/dwell,” though UA shows the pre-Masoretic vowel i.6 UA o for Hebrew o (< proto-Semitic *a) to be seen in later examples also points to Hebrew.
For the data below, the left column generally contains a Hebrew form (an occasional Arabic or other Semitic form will be specified in the notes), and on the right are UA forms. Consider additional lexical similarities:
Heb/Sem | UA | |||
4. | baraq | lightning | berok | lightning7 |
5. | *kilyah/kolyah | kidney | *kali | kidney8 |
6. | katep/katpa | shoulder | *kotpa | shoulder9 |
7. | š”kem/šikm | shoulder | *sïka/siku | shoulder10 |
8. | )adam | man | *otam | man, person11 |
9. | mayim/mem | water | *meme-t | ocean12 |
10. | šippah | smooth, plane off | *sipa | shave, scrape13 |
11. | *siggôb | squirrel | *sikku | squirrel14 |
12. | sippôr | bird | *cipu(ri) | bird15 |
Sound Correspondences
Linguists have found that even though sounds change over time, the changes are not haphazard; sounds change in consistent patterns, such that a sound in one language will quite consistently correspond to a particular sound in a related language. For example, the sound correspondences of English in the Indo-European language family include f < *p (i.e., f is from an original p or reconstructed proto-Indo-European *p); th < *t; and h < *k; and all three show a general trend of stops (p, t, k) becoming fricatives (f, th, h):
English | Latin |
father | pater |
foot | ped- |
three | tres |
thin | tenuis |
hound | kan-is |
heart | kord-is |
hundred | kentum |
Though many details remain to be worked out, a comparison of Hebrew or Semitic with Uto-Aztecan produces a fairly consistent pattern of sound correspondences, which is perhaps the most important linguistic criterion for establishing a relationship between languages. Some of the basic Semitic-UA correspondences are as follows:
Proto-Semitic/ | ||
Arabic | Hebrew | UA |
*b | b | *kw/p16 |
*p | p | *p |
*r | r | y/i |
*( | ( | w/o/u |
*h | h | ho/w/o/u |
*) | ) | w/o/u |
*s | s | c |
*d | s | c |
*z | s | c |
*t | t | c |
*z | z | c |
*´ | z | t |
Similar to the sound correspondence of Latin kw with Greek p in the Indo-European language family, UA kw corresponds to Hebrew b in predictable (dageshed) positions.17 One exception to kw is the Tepiman branch of UA, in which Tepiman b corresponds to UA *kw; thus Tepiman b also corresponds to Hebrew b. Similar to the correspondence of r to y/i in English creoles, Mayan, Athapaskan, and other language families, Hebrew/Semitic r corresponds to PUA *y/i for most UA languages.18 The correspondences for Hebrew *r yield y in most UA languages, r in a few, and d in the Tepiman branch. The vowel i (as in free) is very similar phonologically to y, as realized in repeating the sequence aia quickly, which comes to sound like aya. With those two sound changes in mind (Hebrew b > UA *kw; Hebrew r > UA *y/i), consider the following:
13. | bšl/bašal | boil, ripen | *kwasï | boil, cook, ripen |
14. | dabba (Ar) | keep locked | *cakwa | lock |
dabb/sab (Ar/Heb) | lizard | *cakwa | lizard19 | |
15. | basar | flesh, penis | *kwasiy | tail, penis, flesh20 |
16. | šabber | break | *sakwi/ sakway | break, mess up, ruin21 |
17. | dabber | speak | *tïkwi | tell, say22 |
18. | krr | go in circles, dance | *kiya | have a round dance23 |
19. | mrr | go | *miya | go, travel, run24 |
20. | brr/bar(r) | purify, select land, field grain | *kwiya *kwiya *kwiya | take, keep25 land, earth acorn |
21. | šrq | comb, card | *siyuk | comb26 |
22. | bsr | cut off, enclose | *kwacay | wrap around, to corner27 |
23. | bô | in it | *kwo > ko/bo | in, at28 |
Semitic roots generally consist of three consonants, which employ a variety of voweling patterns for various noun and verb forms. Unless it is a non-qal (not a simple stem) form, only the three consonants will be listed. In the first example of the Hebrew b– UA *kw correspondence, note that Hebrew bšl means both “boil” and “ripen,” and that UA kwasï also means “cook, boil, ripen.” Among the UA correspondences for proto-UA *kw are b in the Tepiman branch, bw in Yq and My, and w in Tr and others, but kw in most UA languages; thus Yq bwase, Od bahi, Tr wasi, and kwasï for most other languages means “cook, boil, ripen.”
As for r > y, note the similar pattern of the Semitic roots ending with double rr consistently matching UA iya (18–20). That the Semitic root brr and the corresponding UA forms kwiya have similar sets of three diverse meanings is worth noting: “choose”/”take”; “land”/”land”; “grain”/”acorn.” A similar semantic correspondence appears in Sem dabba/UA cakwa as both semantic dimensions of “lizard” and “lock/imprison” occur in both language families. Also be aware that Sem and Ar d, s, and z all correspond to Hebrew s and UA c (ts, which is the modern Hebrew pronunciation of s).
The devoicing of Hebrew voiced stops has generally merged them with the voiceless stops in UA: non-dageshed29 Hebrew b and Hebrew p both > UA *p; Hebrew d and Hebrew t both > UA *t; Hebrew g and Hebrew k both > UA *k.
24. | gebîm | locust | *kïpi | locust30 |
25. | daniy (Ar) | low | *tani | below31 |
26. | dayeq | siege-wall | *tïyïqa | wall32 |
27. | daqal (Ar) | palm tree | *taku | palm tree33 |
28. | dqr/daqar | pierce | *tïkiy | cut, stick in34 |
Both of the Hebrew pharyngeals generally cause rounding. The Hebrew voiceless pharyngeal fricative h corresponds to UA ho/hu (usually in initial position) or a round vowel o/u/w without the h quality.
29. | hes | arrow | *huc | arrow35 |
30. | hrk/harak | vi. move | *hoyok | vi. move36 |
31. | hpp | rub, cleanse | *upa | bathe37 |
32. | hmr | smear | *humay | smear38 |
33. | hll | play the pipe | *)ululu | play the flute39 |
34. | )hh (Ar) | cough | *)ohoho | cough40 |
35. | srh | cry, roar | *cayau | cry, yell41 |
36. | smh/yismah | sprout | *icmo-lini | sprout, grow42 |
37. | slh | rush | *coloa | flee43 |
The Hebrew voiced pharyngeal—the Semitic (ain—is a deep back guttural (voiced pharyngeal fricative) that simply yields rounding in UA—o, u, w—like the other pharyngeal.
38. | s(q | cry out | *coak | cry44 |
39. | šg( | be mad | *sikoa | feel envy, suffer45 |
40. | š(( | delight in | *ta-soa | cherish, value, love46 |
41. | bl( | swallow | *kwïlo | taste47 |
42. | rega( | (in a) moment | *rïko | shortly, soon48 |
43. | se(ar/s(r | hair/ be hairy | *sui/suwi | hair49 |
44. | na(ar | boy | *nowi | have a son50 |
45. | ya(ar | forest | *yuy | evergreen tree51 |
46. | d(k | to go out (of fire) | *tuk | go out (of fire)52 |
47. | ps( | to bruise | *pacoa/ paciwi | to bruise53 |
48. | (ly/(alah | go up | *wal | go up54 |
49. | ma(alah | stairs, ascent | i)mola | stairs55 |
50. | (gz | grow old (of women) | *wïgaca | grow old (of women)56 |
51. | d(w/da(a (Ar) | to name | *tïwa | name57 |
Note the consistent pattern that when ( and r are the second and third consonants in Hebrew (43–45), that UA shows uwi/uy (“hair, boy, forest”). Most interesting about 43 is that the root s(r “be hairy” yields a unique semantic combination in three Hebrew words meaning “hair,” “barley” (as “hairy or bearded grain”), and a “buck-goat” (as a hairy animal). Note that the same three semantic categories are contained in the Hopi stem sowi: sowi “hair”; sowiwa “a poor grade of corn” (hairy grain); sowi-t “jackrabbit”; sowinwa “deer” (both as hairy animals). Besides a three-way semantic correspondence, all three consonants agree as expected: š > s; ( > o/w; r > i.
The Semitic )aleph or glottal stop ()) is also prone to rounding effect in UA, as it is in Semitic on occasion (e.g., Ar sa)ala, and V tasawwala).
52. | )arî | lion | wori | mountain lion58 |
8. | )adam | man | *otam | man, person Od, NT, ST |
53. | )îš | man | *wïsi | person Tr59 |
54. | )išt- | woman | *wïti | woman Hp60 |
55. | g)l/ga)al | buy, redeem | *kowa | buy61 |
56. | qr) (Heb/Ar) | call, cry | te-koyoa koyo-n | howl N coyote N62 |
57. | pl)/pala) | be wonderful | *palaw | be pretty Ca |
58. | nb) (Ar) | tell, inform | navo- | learn by hearing Hp |
59. | pe)ah | corner, sideburn | powa/po)a/ po | hair (several languages)63 |
60. | *pa)r (Ar) | mouse | puwe-/pu)i- | mouse64 |
61. | )egôz | nut | *woko | pinion pinenut and tree (several) |
62. | ya-)amîn | he believes | yawamin | believe Sr |
63. | ya-)amîn-ô | he believes it | yawayno | believe it Gb |
64. | kam) (Ar) kama)atu(m) (Ug) | truffletruffle | kamo)-ni kamwah | sweet potato N; sweet potato Cr |
65. | tirmania | truffle | tïmna/tïmön | potato Hp65 |
The two forms for believe (62–63) are especially striking. First of all, seven segments (vowels or consonants) are present in the Hebrew form—four consonants and three vowels. All seven segments (of the third person masc. sg. Hebrew form ya-)amîn) match exactly as expected in the Sr form (yawamin). With four consonants and three vowels, the probability of a word as lengthy as the Sr form, in light of 12 proto-UA consonants and 5 PUA vowels, aligning with the Hebrew form by chance is one in two and a half million (1/12 x 5 x 12 x 5 x 12 x 5 x 12 = 1/2,592,000). The Gb form lost only m (yawain < yawamin), but profoundly compelling for a Hebrew connection is its slightly different meaning: “believe it,” instead of “believe.” To add a third person singular object to a verb in Hebrew, –o is suffixed, which yields “he believes him/it.” And in Gb we have both the meaning (“believe it”) and exactly the Hebrew suffix (-o) to match the meaning that includes an object. Fossilized as the morphology is, I might mention that most of the discernible Semitic morphology in UA is fossilized rather than productive. And as examples of fossilized Hebrew morphology, the Sr and Gb pair (62 and 63) are astounding in themselves.
Note also the two Near East words for truffle that are similar to UA words for potato. Tirmania is not a Semitic word, but is a Near East word for truffle. Though the truffle and potato are not exactly the same thing, they are both fleshy edible nodules appendaged to a root system growing underground, and UA has two words for potato similar to two Mediterranean words for truffle.
Somewhat similar to the correspondence of English t and German ss in foot/fuss and street/strasse, Hebrew emphatic t and emphatic s (see the appendix) both generally correspond to UA c, sometimes s, though s/c alternations are common within UA itself also. Following are examples of emphatic t:
66. | tll (Ar) | sprinkle/ drizzle | cölölö | sprinkle/ start raining (Hp) |
67. | )abattîh/ bittih (Ar) | melon | baci | pumpkin (Tr) |
68. | twy/ tawa (Ar) | spin (thread) | cawa | spin (thread)66 |
69. | t(m | taste, eat | cu)mi | sip (Wr) |
70. | hût/xayt | thread, twine | wic | string67 |
71. | hatab (Ar) | firewood | *ucakwi | resin, pitch68 |
72. | matteh | branch, rod | ko-maci | firewood69 |
73. | tabal | dip s.th. | cakwa- | soak s.th. (N) |
Hebrew initial r corresponds to UA t in initial position, except in Tr, in which it remained r:
74. | r)y/ra)ah | see | *tïwa | see, find (several languages) |
75. | rabab/rbb | shoot (an arrow) | *tokwa | snap (of bow)70 |
76. | r(m | to thunder | *tom | thunder, cloud, winter71 |
77. | rbt (Ar) | to tie, bind | *tapic | to tie72 |
78. | raqîa( | sky | *tuku | sky (several languages) |
79. | rš( | bad, wicked | *tïsïw | cause/do bad73 |
80. | rajul (Ar) | man | *tïhoy | man74 |
81. | roš | head | *toci | head75 |
82. | )arnab-ôt | rabbits | *tavo-t | rabbit(s) (several languages) |
The velar and uvular stops—k, q, and g—often reduce to glottal stop ()) or nothing in initial position or in consonant clusters.
83. | kanap | wing | *)anap | wing76 |
84. | kinnîm | gnats | *)ani | mosquito77 |
85. | geled/gild | skin | )eld | skin78 |
86. | gll/golla | roll/ ball | )ola/nola | ball79 |
87. | qarôb | near,soon | )ayobe | soon (Tr) |
88. | qereb | midst, inside | *)ïrap | in the middle of80 |
89. | makteš | mortar, grinding stone | *ma)ta- | grinding stone, metate |
90. | ka/kî | you, your sg. | *)ï | you, your sg. |
91. | -kem, -kum | you, your pl. | *)ïm | you, your pl. |
The term for grinding stone (89) is found throughout UA languages; in fact, the Aztec word mena-n is the source for metate, borrowed into Spanish and English. Though *mata is the usual reconstruction, the forms Tr ma)ta, Wr mahta, Od maccud, and My matta all suggest a consonant cluster, with Tr showing something very much like k, since k in a cluster becomes a glottal stop very often, not only in this connection, but in English (dictate > di)tet), Polynesian, and many other languages. In addition to the word for mortar or grinding stone matching quite well, two verbs in UA languages match the Hebrew perfect and imperfect, respectively. Hebrew makteš is a nominal form from the verb ktš “pound, bray, grind”: Aramaic ktaš; Hebrew kataš. The imperfect stem in Hebrew is –ktôš and no less than 17 UA languages have forms showing *tus “grind,” which is exactly what we would expect with the general rising of vowels (though Hebrew o < *u of proto-Semitic) and the disappearance of k in a cluster (-ktôš > )tus > tus), as it also disappeared in the noun forms (makteš > ma)ta). In addition, consider Yq kítte “grinding flour” and Yq kíttasu “make into pieces.” Though this stem does not exhibit the qittel form in the Masoretic text, the Yq forms match qittel forms of the perfect.
Consider the likelihood of all this matching by chance:
Heb | kataš/ *kitteš | grind | Yq | kitte/ kittasu | grind, smash |
Heb | -ktôš | grind (impf) | UA | *tus | grind (in 17 UA languages) |
Heb | makteš | mortar | UA | *ma)ta | mortar or grinding stone |
The probability of three separate UA forms matching three very different and highly specific morphological patterns built on the same Semitic stem, all by chance, with corresponding meanings, seems slim.
Pronouns
In any comparative study, pronouns are an important consideration. Elaborating on the second person pronouns cited above (90 and 91), we note that the UA second person pronouns correspond to the suffix (object and possessive) pronouns of Hebrew. Consider a more complete array of forms:
singular | plural | |
Tb | imbi | imbumu |
Ch | ïmi | mïmi |
Hp | )ïm | )ïma |
Yq | )empo | )eme)e |
SP | immi- | mwïmmwi- |
Cp | i-/e-/e)e | imi-/eme-/em)em |
Ca | )e | )em |
Hp | )ï- | )ïmi- (possessive pronouns) |
Cr | mu)e | mu)en |
Yq | -a)e | -a)em (enclitic pronouns) |
My | –)e | –)em (enclitic pronouns) |
Heb/Sem | -ka/-k(i) | -kem/*-kum |
Given k > ), those UA languages below the line show a similar singular and plural distinction as Hebrew. The others appear to correlate with something similar to what happened in English; just as English pl. you replaced sg. thou as second person singular, such that sg. and pl. you in English both derive from what was originally only plural, likewise half the UA languages (above the line) appear to derive both their second person sg. and pl. forms from the plural as seen by an abundance of m, which signifies plural in Hebrew (and UA). However, some UA languages—those below the line—appear to have maintained the singular-plural distinction, as seen by lack of final m in the singular forms, but inclusion of final m‘s in the plural forms.
Though UA second person pronouns generally parallel Hebrew suffix pronouns, one UA language shows both the independent/subject pronouns and the above object/possessive pronouns for second person plural. Consider the Tarahumara forms:
92. | ||
Ar/PrSem | )antum (indep prn) | -kum (obj/suffix prn) |
Heb | )attem (indep prn) | -kem (obj/suffix prn) |
Ar/PrSem | -tum (sbj prn on pf v) | |
Heb | -tem (sbj prn on pf v) | |
Tarahumara | tumuhe (sbj prn) | emi (dative/obj prn) |
The above are a profound match of subject pronouns (left column) and object pronouns (right column) for Semitic and Tarahumara. In addition to the subject pronoun suffixes for perfect verb forms, Hebrew also has prefixes on imperfect verb forms, and the second person singular Hebrew prefix is identical with the Nahuatl second person singular prefix (ti-):
93. | Hebrew | Nahuatl | ||
verb stem | -rbas | lie down | -koc | sleep |
you sg. | ti-rbas | you sg. lie down | ti-koc | you sg. sleep |
The above verb, by the way, also corresponds. The consonant cluster in Hebrew causes a dageshed (doubled) b, which in turn corresponds to UA kw, and r (which is y/i in UA) after i is basically invisible, and the vowel reduces or assimilates to the kw, as happens often in UA itself. Thus Hebrew ti-rbas > *ti-kwac > *ti-kwc > N ti-koc.
Unlike other UA languages, whose pronouns agree more with Hebrew independent and suffix pronouns, Nahuatl singular pronouns parallel Semitic imperfective verb prefixes, as if derived from a verb form:
94. | NW Sem sg. | NW Sem pl. | Nahuatl | |||
1st person | )e-/ )a- | I (verb) | ni-/na- | we (verb) | ne)wa | I |
2nd “ | ti-/ ta- | you (verb) | ti-/ta- | te)wa | you | |
3rd “ | yi-/ ya- | he (verbs) | yi-/ya- | ye)wa | he |
Note the pattern of n as first person, t as second person, and y as a third person consonant in both Nahuatl and Semitic, though the 1st person singular verb prefix in Semitic is an exception. Moving from second person to first person pronouns, consider some UA first person singular pronouns (I, me, my):
95. | Independent (I) | suffix (object and possessive: me, my) |
Heb | )anî, )anokî | -nî, -î |
Ch | nïï | |
SP | nï | |
Tb | nik | |
Hp | nï) | i- |
Ca | ne) | |
Od | )ani | |
Tr | ne |
96. One other first person pronoun in Tr is highly specific. In addition to independent pronouns, subject-of-verb prefixes, and object/possessive suffixes, Hebrew also has nonaffixed object pronouns in the form of )oti “me,” )oto “him,” )ota “her,” etc. Though I have not noticed any of the others, the first—)oti “me”—is quite comparable to the Tr accusative (object) pronoun of Tr ti “me,” only missing the first segment (o), but Tr tends to lack first segments in comparative UA as well.
Many third person pronouns appear similar as well:
97. | sg. he/she/him, his | pl. they/them/their |
Semitic | hu/huwa/hi/hiya/-ô | hem/hum/-am |
SP | unwa | humwi |
Yq | hu (that) | hume (those), )am, -ame |
Ca | he-, hi- | hem- |
Hp | –)am |
These four languages represent four separate branches of UA—Numic, Sonoran, Takic, and Hopi respectively. The functions of third person pronouns in UA languages are often served by demonstratives, thus eliminating older third person pronouns; however, many of those demonstrative pronouns (that/those) are similar to Semitic third person pronouns. As Langacker notes, the pronominal systems of UA “have undergone extensive modification, so that definitive reconstruction will have to await extensive research.”81 That is exactly correct; nevertheless, numerous Semitic-looking elements are to be found in UA pronominal systems, though mixed considerably with other, non-Semitic characteristics—thus again the conclusion of substantial mixing of some kind.
Vav-consecutive Fossilized in Nahuatl
98. A partial and oversimplified explanation of the vav-consecutive in Hebrew is that a prefixed wa- changes imperfect verb stems to past. Most Nahuatl verbs form the past tense by prefixing o- and dropping the last vowel:
penawa | undress | o-penaw- | undressed |
(if not 3rd sg., insert pron.) | o-ti-penaw | you undressed | |
neki | want | o-nek- | wanted |
pawia | chew | o-pawi- | chewed |
posoni | boil | o-poson- | boiled, bubbled (of liquid) |
In Hebrew, the jussive is used with the vav-consecutive, and the jussive also drops existing final vowels in Hebrew and Arabic, as do the Nahuatl verbs with prefixed o-:
Heb prefix | Heb wa- + juss | Ar indic | Ar juss |
yišbeh take captive | wa-yišb took captive | yaktubu write | yaktub |
For wa- to become o- is natural enough. Consider Spanish ojalá “would that” from Arabic wa-ša)a-allah “and/if God wills.” Therefore, the Hebrew vav-consecutive and the Nahuatl past tense have these things in common: they both prefix rounded elements (wa- and o-), then a pronominal prefix, then the stem, then they both drop final vowels, and they both change an imperfect stem to perfect (loosely stated).
99. Another curious set in UA which parallels Hebrew morphology has to do with the Hebrew root nky/naka “to smite.” This stem does not appear in the simplest or qal form in Hebrew much, but is very commonly used in the hiqtîl and huqtal in Hebrew. Forms parallel to the Hebrew participles of hiqtîl and huqtal are also common words in UA languages.
The Hebrew participles are makke (< *mankey) “smiter, smiting” and mukke “(one) smitten.” One of the most pervasive stems in UA is *muki “die, be sick, dead” found in no less than 13 UA languages (UACS #128a), which matches the passive (huqtal participle mukke) both phonologically and semantically. In addition are words in several UA languages reconstructing to *mek “kill” (UACS #128d) and *mak “hit” (UACS #233), which again parallel the Hebrew active participle (Hebrew makke), both phonologically and semantically. The Cahuilla pair show both in the same language: Ca -muk- “get sick, die”; Ca -mek- “kill.” (Again note the general rising of vowels in the changes from Semitic to UA.)
Another dimension of the Hebrew verb is “punish, send judgment.” In light of that compare the Nahuatl na- form: N miki “die”; N na-miki “bring upon oneself, incur a fine or punishment.”
The similarities (lexical, morphological, and semantic combinations) between UA and Semitic number about 1000. Therefore, this brief summary contains only 10% of them. One question that naturally comes to the mind of an Hebraist or Semitist is the lack of some of the basic words, such as šemeš “sun” and yad “hand.” Three possibilities come to mind. First, as emphasized throughout this paper, UA is not solely descended from Hebrew in any sense, but rather appears to have a Northwest Semitic element that has mixed heavily with non-Semitic elements. Second, UA could be more a Mulekite base with a Lehite overlay (both in addition to whatever else). We know next to nothing about the composition of the Mulekite group. We do not know whether they built a ship or hired one. If the latter, the crew or those aboard were likely an international mix—perhaps Phoenicians, Greeks, and Arabs—and if so, the Mulekite language within a generation could well have been a creole or hybrid of who knows how many languages. That would be one possible explanation among many for the Nephites’ inability to understand them after only four centuries. The river Sidon being named after the Phoenician capital Sidon speaks for a Phoenician element among them, since Sidon is hardly part of the ancient Israelite domain. Third, we do not know Lehi’s nor Ishmael’s dialect; that eventual knowledge is bound to be surprising in some ways. Nibley elaborates the Arab-like qualities of the Lehi-Ishmael party: that Ishmael’s name is reminiscent of the father of the Arabs; that Manasseh, of all the tribes, mixed and associated with the Arabs more frequently than any of the twelve tribes; and the Arabic nature of names like Lehi, Laman, Lemuel, and Sam.82 Nibley’s observations and the surprising proportion of Arabic vocabulary in UA are mutually consistent with each other.
Returning to the whereabouts of some basic Hebrew vocabulary, a look at UA occasionally suggests that some basic vocabulary could have been replaced by semantic extensions of other Semitic vocabulary. For example, the common Semitic word laila “night” is not found. However, the UA word for “night” (found in many UA languages) is tuk, and it also means “black,” “dark,” and the “fire went out.” Hebrew d(k means “to go out” (of a fire), and phonologically the match is exactly as expected: devoicing of d > t; round vowel for the pharyngeal (ain; and k. When the “fire goes out” at night, it is then “dark, black, night,” and the word from Hebrew d(k appears to be the source of UA tuk “night” (as well as “dark, black, fire go out”). Many are the examples of such extensions of some Hebrew words into new semantic domains.
Another array of curiosities involves the UA words for “man.” All four of them are traceable to Semitic, but they occur in exactly the opposite frequency typical of Hebrew. The most frequent word for “man” in Hebrew is )îš, which is found in only Tr wesi (<*wïsi) and only in a certain phrase with a negative, meaning “no one, no man,” which is one of the typical uses of Hebrew )îš. The second most common word in Hebrew is )adam, and that is found in about five UA languages (*otam). (Keep in mind for both )îš and )adam, that the initial )aleph or glottal stop is a source of rounding in UA.) Least common in Hebrew is zakar “male/man” (Ar dakar, Aram dakar), while most common in UA is *taka “man.” Hebrew z is a merger of two proto-Semitic consonants, *z and *´, that appear in Arabic as z and ´, and in Aramaic as z and d. Interestingly, some evidence suggests that UA also distinguishes these as UA *c and *t, respectively; thus the stop t in UA correspondes to proto-Semitic ´.
Heb/Sem | UA | |||
100. | zakar (Ar dakar; Aram dakar) | male, man | *taka | man83 |
101. | z )eb (Ar d)b) | wolf | *tï)ïp | wolf84 |
102. | zaqan/ziqn- (Akk) | chin, beard | *tï)n | mouth85 |
Fourth and most curious is UA *tïhoy “man,” suggestive of the most common Ar word for man: rajul.86
Egyptian
Of great interest are some UA lexemes that may match Egyptian. For example, both the Hebrew word for “lion” and the Egyptian word for “lion” appear in UA languages. Keep in mind that the glottal stop ()) or Semitic )aleph corresponds to UA w or some round vowel, since both of these words show that correspondence:
52. | Heb | )arî | lion | UA | *wori | mountain lion |
103. | Eg | m)i | lion | UA | *mawiya | mountain lion |
Cpt | mui | lion |
Ancient Egyptian, like many ancient Near Eastern languages, exhibited only consonants (i or y recorded as a consonant). The UA word mawiya “mountain lion” is found in several UA languages, and it shows all three consonants of the Egyptian word very nicely. Also of interest is that in Coptic—a later form of Egyptian in which vowels were written—one can see that the glottal stop also resulted in rounding (u) as is typical in UA: Coptic mui “lion.” Though not altogether consistent, the same consonants that yield rounding in the change from Hebrew to UA often exhibit a similar tendency in the change from ancient Egyptian to Coptic:
Egyptian | Coptic | ||
hbs | clothe | hoves | cover, clothe |
htp | happy, at peace, | hotep | be reconciled, |
set | set (of sun) | ||
s) | back | soi | back |
s(y | sand | so | sand |
() | great | o/o | great |
(nh | live/life | oneh | live |
(rf | bag, enclose | orev | enclose |
However, exceptions also exist: Eg r( “sun,” Cpt re “sun.” Consider other similarities between Egyptian and UA:
104. | Eg | nmi | travel, traverse | *nïmi | wander, go about |
105. | Eg | rn | young (of animals) | Tr rana | brood, litter, child |
(Remember that initial r is UA t, except it remained r in Tr.) | |||||
106. | Eg | nb | all, every | Tr nepi | a lot, too much |
107. | Eg | r( | sun | *tawe | sun, day |
108. | Cpt tevet | fish | *pa-top/ pa-tap | fish (pa=water) |
For items 105 and 107, remember that Tr r = Hebrew r = UA *t in initial position; therefore, the Tr form rawe “day” is equivalent to UA *tawe found in Eu, Yq, My, Wr, and Hp, all of which match nicely the Egyptian word r( “sun,” with the expected w for the pharyngeal (ain. In regard to item 108, we might mention that v is an alternate form of p in both Coptic and UA.
Other Egyptian examples exist, but these are sufficient to show that if UA was, in part, a Lehite language, then a certain amount of Egyptian vocabulary worked its way into the spoken language, just as Latin words entered English via Latin as a liturgical or written language of religious record. The proportions of Egyptian are not great compared to the amount of Hebrew, as we would expect; nevertheless, any Egyptian vocabulary at all is significant.
Book of Mormon Peoples
One may also wonder if there is any evidence in UA to suggest that UA peoples may be in part remnants of Book of Mormon peoples. From a number of possibilities, consider two.
- Hopi masaw/masawï “supreme deity, supernatural judge” fits nicely the three consonants of maššîh or maššîah (Messiah). The final h is the pharyngeal h, which yields w or rounding in UA; therefore, outside of the missing vowel i, all else and especially the three consonants are as expected.
- The word for Nephite in Hebrew would be nepi/nefi, depending on how much the Nephite language was subject to the spirantization (of *p to f) evident by the time the Masoretes imposed their dialect or allophonic variants on the text. UA languages are split: some show a similar spirantization of Hebrew *p to v rather than f, while others retain p. This would suggest that the spirantization evident in the Masoretic dialect may not have occurred when this American dialect of Hebrew left Palestine, since some UA languages do not show it, but that a similar (though slightly different) spirantization occurred later in some UA languages also. Either way, the intervocalic form of Hebrew p is p or v in UA, not f as in the Masoretic dialect, though Egyptian has both p and f. In addition, the –ite ending of English biblical nationalities is a mistaken adoption of the feminine adjectival ending showing –t and is incorrect. In Hebrew, words featuring persons of an ethnic group simply use the suffix –i (as the vowel in free); for example, a Moabite is moabi, an Ammonite is ammoni, an Israelite is israeli, as said of Israelis in modern Israel today. Thus a Nephite would be nepi/nepiy/nepi (a long vowel at the end, however one chooses to represent it). Plural Nephites would contain the plural suffix –îm or earlier –ima, and would thus be nepiyyîm or with a typical reduction of that long string of fairly identical high front vowels (i/y) and the older ending as is found in UA (-ima), we would have nepima or nepima. Pima happens to be the tribal name of two UA groups in the Tepiman branch of UA and is missing only the first syllable ne– of what would otherwise be the expected plural form of Nephites in Hebrew.87 Another name for a group in the same branch (Tepiman) of UA is nevome. Remember that the final a of our UA reconstruction *-ima is often e or ï in most UA languages. Likewise, for a vowel to assimilate to a round vowel (i > o) when adjacent to one bilabial is common enough, and here i is caught between two bilabials (v and m), which would make the change i > o even more likely; furthermore, v is a form of UA *p between vowels. In essence, the UA tribal name Nevome is easily derivable from Nepima (Nephites).
Though it is too early to say definitively, the above tribal names (e.g., nevome < *nepima) and several other factors suggest a possibility worth considering: could the UA peoples be in part surviving Nephite or Mulekite populations in the land northward or in northern extensions of the land northward, since the UA tribes form a fairly contiguous chain from Mexico City northward up through western Mexico to the US Southwest? Of course, few, if any, UA areas would be the areas that the Book of Mormon authors referred to as having “large bodies of water and many rivers” and “houses of cement” (Helaman 3:3–4, 7; 6:6; 7:1); nevertheless, some of them, at least, could be northern extensions of the areas spoken of. In 55 B.C. some 5,400 families departed out of Zarahemla for the land northward, and Hagoth built ships to transport more to the land northward (Alma 63:3–4). Nearly a decade later in 46 B.C. “An exceedingly great many . . . went forth unto the land northward. . . . And they did travel to an exceedingly great distance” (Helaman 3:3–4). Consider the following factors:
- If Mesoamerica is the area of Book of Mormon history, as proposed by Sorenson88 and most Latter-day Saint archaeologists, north of that is a fairly unbroken continuum of UA speaking groups stretching from Mexico City northward to Southern California and the US Southwest. The Aztecs arrived (or was it returned?) well after Book of Mormon times, but what of the closely related Cora, Huichol, and other UA languages just north of Nahuatl-speaking areas? Even if the Mexico City area was inhabited late by UA speakers, points just north have long been UA areas.
- Hagoth’s ships launched into the west sea to sail to the land northward (Alma 63:5), and it is precisely the western coastal and mountainous areas of western Mexico that UA peoples inhabit. The existence of a regular timber-shipping industry along the western coasts of a land northward (Helaman 3:10) from anywhere in Mesoamerica would have the western coast of Mexico, the habitat of UA speakers, as a likely candidate for the west coast of the land northward.
- The Anasazi culture of the US Southwest includes UA peoples (Hopi) and other UA relatives (Tanoan pueblos in New Mexico), and archaeologically the Anasazi appear about the time of Christ, which date accords well with Hagoth and the times of this northward expansion.
- The Pima and O’odham of Arizona (UA groups) are the most likely candidates as the continuation of the Hohokam culture, though that is yet debated. The Hohokam are known for their connections with Mexico, though they date a little earlier than the Anasazi, perhaps 300 B.C.
- Some Uto-Aztecanists suggest that the linguistic center of gravity for the northern half of the UA language family is near the California-Arizona border just above the mouth of the Colorado River that empties into the Gulf of Baja California (see fig. 1).89 If some Nephite ships happened to sail farther northward than usual, keeping near the coastline, they would likely go inside the tongue of Baja California, and the ultimate destination would be the top of the Gulf of Baja California, near the point of origin of the northern UA languages.
- No matter who built the houses of cement, nearly all the Southern UA languages have a common word for “adobe” (sami). The word adobe was not in the 1830 edition of Webster’s Dictionary, and Joseph Smith may not have been familiar with the term adobe. If not, his use of cement may refer to or at least partly include adobe.90 And if that is so, could not the pueblo builders, who anciently were as much in Mexico as the US Southwest, be northern extensions of those who built houses of cement?
- Wilford Woodruff expressed a view that the pueblo builders of New Mexico were in part Nephites.91
- The rugged mountains of western Mexico appear to be the homeland of the Southern UA groups. If the Nephite-Mulekite populations were in these mountains around A.D. 400 when Lamanites were striving to hunt down and kill all Nephites, the ruggedness of the terrain would be a wonderful protection and provide thousands of hiding places. Or this area may have been beyond reach of, or not involved in, all that happened through the final destruction of the Nephites. The linguistic evidence suggests that it is from these areas that many of the Southern UA languages appear to have spread.
- And last, but hardly least, are a few UA tribal names such as nevome (< *nepima “Nephites”) that suggest the UA peoples may be in part remnants of Book of Mormon peoples.
Conclusions
In conclusion, UA as a language family exhibits more similarities with Hebrew than could be attributed to coincidence; nevertheless, that Hebrew element is obviously mixed with other language elements very different from Hebrew. The Hebrew features, along with other factors, combine to suggest that the formation or spread of UA peoples may have involved Book of Mormon peoples in part, and, more specifically, perhaps Nephite or Mulekite populations that had spread northward from lands more central to the Book of Mormon record.
A more balanced approach to Book of Mormon language research could be immensely beneficial in the long run. Hebrew, Egyptian, and the English translation have thus far been the sole focus of Latter-day Saint scholars publishing on Book of Mormon language matters. Exclusive concern with those three areas of interest has left Latter-day Saint scholarship at an impasse on many points, while the huge arena of Native American languages remains largely untouched by Latter-day Saint scholars, though obviously these languages must be dealt with eventually. While the English text has yielded important insights to our analysts, only when Latter-day Saint scholars delve into Native American languages as well can we consider a comprehensive approach to Book of Mormon language matters to be underway. The neglected dimension of research (in Native Americana) could well prove to be the key to many questions impossible to answer by means of Hebrew, Egyptian, and the English text alone.
The hints and leads exist, but they must be searched and worked rigorously. Responsible linguistic investigation of Native American languages in conjunction with Near Eastern languages should be a natural realm of research for Latter-day Saint scholarship and interests. We claim and proclaim knowledge of some ancient American groups, yet our void of attention to Native American languages for a century and a half subsequent to those claims could border on embarrassment if allowed to continue. A people’s language is a window to their past and is often the most voluminous repository of hard data relevant to their origins and past. In light of the potential of Native American languages, it seems time for a change—a change from overlooking them to looking them over in linguistically competent ways. True, the required research investment would be considerable. Comparable to the difference between miles and light-years, the effort would better be measured in units of linguist-lives than in man-hours. Nevertheless,?rather than all interested scholars dipping for linguistic depth in the phrasings of the English translation, would that a few explore the ocean of Native American languages and acquire the necessary background to enter this forgotten realm of research and help void the void.
Appendix
Orthography and Pronunciation
The phonetic representation used in this paper is fairly standard linguistic phonology. The phonetic symbols are as follows:
Vowels
a as in father, saw, rod e roughly as in fame, say, raid i as in fee, see, reed o as in foe, so, road u as in Sue, rude ï high central vowel, not in English, a high schwa “ the schwa or midcentral vowel, as in but, cut, come ö midfront rounded vowel as in German and Hopi Long vowels will be represented with a macron as in a, i Nasalized vowels will be underlined: a.
The vowels of Masoretic Hebrew—segol E and cere e—will both be represented as e, since both are substantial alterations of earlier Semitic vowels (i and a usually), and it is pointless to be painfully specific regarding Masoretic vowels anyway, since many of them are phonological variants of a late dialect that come from only three vowels—a, i, u—in pre-Hebrew or Northwest Semitic.
Vowels are described according to the tongue’s position in the mouth when pronounced; thus i is high-front, the schwa “ is mid-central, etc.
front | central | back | |
high | i | ï | u |
mid | e | “ | o |
low | a |
Consonants
Most consonants are pronounced more or less as in English; nevertheless, a full presentation of consonants follows:
bi- labial | dental | alv- eolar | palatal | velar | uvular | pharyn- geal | glottal | |
stop vcless | p | t | k | q | ) | |||
voiced | b | d | g | |||||
affric. vcless | c,s (Hb) | c | ||||||
voiced | j | |||||||
fric. vcless | f | Ø,t (Hb) | s | š | x | h | h | |
voiced | v | ´ | z | z | g | ( | ||
nasals | m | n | n | |||||
liquids | r,l |
Explanations and additional sounds
The c is a ts sound, very common in UA, as in hats.
The palato-alveolars have the hashmark:
c = ch as in chop; j as in judge; š = sh as in shop; z = zh as in azure.
The pharyngeals of Semitic are represented by:
h voiceless pharyngeal fricative (as opposed to English h);
( voiced pharyngeal fricative, the Semitic (ain, as in Sa(udi (Arabia.
The velar nasal n as in sing.
The dental fricatives: ´ as in breathe and they, and t as in breath and think.
n is the lateral stop tl of Nahuatl, which corresponds to UA *t.
t is the emphatic t of Hebrew and Semitic.
Hebrew emphatic s is a merger of three proto-Semitic consonants that are still distinguished in Arabic; that is, Arabic s _, d _, and z _ all correspond to Hebrew s.
Three s sounds in Semitic are all distinguishable in Hebrew; however, they all merged to simply s in UA:
proto-Semitic | Hebrew | Arabic | |
s1 | š | shin | s |
s2 | s | sin | š |
s3 | s | samech | s |
The beged-kafat letters, which spirantized in non-dageshed positions in the Masoretic dialect (b > v, p > f, etc.) will not show that spirantization in this paper, since it is not a feature of proto- or original Hebrew and may not apply to other dialects of ancient Hebrew. Some of the UA languages show similar spirantization; others do not. Likewise, Arabic f will also be represented in its original form *p since that is how it remains in UA.
Abbreviations other than those listed in the text
Akk = Akkadian Ar = Arabic Aram = Aramaic Cpt = Coptic Eg = Egyptian Heb = Hebrew impf = imperfect indep = independent masc = masculine n = noun obj = object pf = perfect pl = plural prn = pronoun PrSem = proto-Semitic PUA = proto-Uto-Aztecan rcp = reciprocal rfl = reflexive sbj = subject sg = singular Sem = Semitic s.o. = someone s.th. = something UA = Uto-Aztecan UACS = Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets Ug = Ugaritic v = verb vi = verb intransitive vt = verb transitive
Works Consulted
- Andrews, J. Richard. Introduction to Classical Nahuatl. Austin: University of Texas Press, 1975.
- Anonymous. Arte y vocabulario de la lengua dohema, heve, o eudeve, ed. C. W. Penington. Mexico: Universidad Nacional Autonoma de Mexico, Instituto de Investigaciones Filologicas, 1981.
- Badger, George Percy. An English-Arabic Lexicon. Beirut: Librairie du Liban, 1967.
- Bascom, Burton. “Northern Tepehuan.” In Studies in Uto-Aztecan Grammar, vol. 3 of Uto-Aztecan Grammatical Sketches, ed. Ronald W. Langacker. Arlington: Summer Institute of Linguistics and University of Texas at Arlington, 1982.
- Bickerton, Derek. Roots of Language. Ann Arbor: Karoma, 1981.
- Joshua. A Grammar of Biblical Hebrew. Wiesbaden: Otto Harrassowitz, 1976.
- Brambila, David. Diccionario Tarahumara-Castellano. Mexico City: Editorial Buena Prensa, 1976.
- Bright, William. A Luiseño Dictionary. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1968.
- Brown, Francis, S. R. Driver, and Charles A. Briggs. A Hebrew and English Lexicon of the Old Testament. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 1979.
- Campbell, Lyle. Quichean Linguistic Prehistory. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1977.
- Caplice, Richard. Introduction to Akkadian, 3rd rev. ed. Rome: Biblical Institute Press, 1988.
- Collard, Howard, and Elisabeth Scott Collard. Castellano-Mayo, Mayo-Castellano. Mexico: Instituto Linguistico de Verano, 1974.
- Dakin, Karen. “Phonological Changes in Nahuatl: The Tense, Aspect, Number Systems.” International Journal of American Linguistics 45/1 (1979): 48–71.
- Dayley, Jon P. Tümpisa (Panamint) Shoshone Dictionary. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1989.
- Dunnigan, Timothy. “Lower Pima.” In Southwest, ed. Alfonso Ortiz. Vol. 10 of Handbook of North American Indians, gen. ed. William C. Sturtevant, 217–29. Washington: Smithsonian Institution, 1983.
- Escalante, Fernando. Native Speaker of Arizona Yaqui and a Ph.D. candidate in linguistics at the University of Arizona.
- Faulkner, Raymond O. A Concise Dictionary of Middle Egyptian. London: Griffith Institute at the Oxford University Press, 1962.
- Gardiner, Alan H. Egyptian Grammar. London: Oxford University Press, 1969.
- Gesenius, Wilhelm. Gesenius’ Hebrew Grammar, ed. E. Kautzsch, trans. A. E. Cowley. 2nd ed. Oxford: Clarendon, 1910.
- Givon, Talmy. Ute Dictionary. Ignacio, CO: Ute Press, 1979.
- Heimsch, Charles. “Truffle.” In The Encyclopaedia Americana, 27:170. New York: Americana, 1962.
- Jane H., and Rosinda Nolasquez. Mulu’wetam: The First People. Cupeño Oral History and Language. Banning, CA: Malki Museum Press, 1973.
- Kenneth C. Interim computer printout of a Serrano Dictionary. 1994.
- Hoijer, Harry. Studies in Athapaskan Languages. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1963.
- Huehnergard, John. Ugaritic Vocabulary in Syllabic Transcription. Atlanta: Scholars Press, 1987.
- Johnson, Jean B. El Idioma Yaqui. Mexico City: Instituto Nacional de Antropologia de Historia, 1962.
- Kalectaca, Milo. Lessons in Hopi, ed. Ronald W. Langacker. Tucson: University of Arizona Press, 1978.
- Kartunnen, Francis. An Analytical Dictionary of Nahuatl. Austin: Unversity of Texas Press, 1983.
- Lambdin, Thomas O. Introduction to Sahidic Coptic. Macon, GA: Mercer University Press, 1983.
- Edward W. An Arabic-English Lexicon. London: Williams and Norgate, 1863–1893.
- Langacker, Ronald W. An Overview of Uto-Aztecan Grammar. Vol. 1 of Studies in Uto-Aztecan Grammar. Arlington: Summer Institute of Linguistics and University of Texas at Arlington, 1977.
- Lionnet, Andres. Los Elementos de la Lengua Cahita. Mexico City: Universidad Nacional Autonona de Mexico, 1977.
- Lionnet, Andres. El Idioma Tubar y los Tubares. Mexico City: Universidad Iberoamericana, 1978.
- Lionnet, Andres. “Relaciones Internas de la Rama Sonorense.” Amerindia 10 (1985): 25–58.
- Mathiot, Madaleine. A Dictionary of Papago Usage. Tucson: The University of Arizona Press, 1976.
- Miller, Wick R. Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1967.
- Miller, Wick R. Newe Natekwinappeh: Shoshone Stories and Dictionary, ed. Jesse D. Jennings. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press, 1972.
- Miller, Wick R. “Uto-Aztecan Languages.” In Southwest, ed. Alfonso Ortiz. Vol. 10 of Handbook of North American Indians, gen. ed. William C. Sturtevant. Washington: Smithsonian Institution, 1983.
- Miller, Wick R. “Computerized Data Base for Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets.” 1988.
- Miller, Wick R. “Guarijio: Gramatica, Textos y Vocabulario.” 1989.
- Moscati, Sabatino, ed. An Introduction to the Comparative Study of the Semitic Languages. Wiesbaden: Otto Harrassowitz, 1964.
- Nibley, Hugh. “Lehi and the Arabs.” In An Approach to the Book of Mormon, 3rd ed., 71–83. Salt Lake City: Deseret Book and FARMS, 1988.
- Oxford English Dictionary. Oxford: Clarendon, 1933.
- Press, Margaret L. Chemehuevi: A Grammar and Lexicon. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1980.
- Robinson, Lila Wistrand, and James Armagost. Comanche Dictionary and Grammar. Arlington: Summer Institute of Linguistics and University of Texas at Arlington, 1990.
- Sapir, Edward. “Southern Paiute: A Shoshonean Language.” American Academy of Arts and Sciences, Proceedings 65 (1930): 1–296.
- Saxton, Dean, Lucille Saxton, and Susie Enos. Dictionary: Papago and Pima to English, English to Papago and Pima. Tucson: University of Arizona Press, 1969 and 1983.
- Seaman, P. David. Hopi Dictionary. Flagstaff: Dept. of Anthropology, Northern Arizona University, 1985.
- Seiler, Hansjakob. Cahuilla Grammar. Banning, CA: Malki Museum Press, 1977.
- Seiler, Hansjakob, and Kojiro Hioki. Cahuilla Dictionary. Banning, CA: Malki Museum Press, 1979.
- Simeon, Remi. Diccionario de la Lengua Nahuatl or Mexicana. Paris: Imprimerie Nationale, 1885; Mexico City: Siglo Veintiuno Editores, 1977.
- Sorenson, John L. An Ancient American Setting for the Book of Mormon Salt Lake City: Deseret Book and FARMS, 1985.
- Sorenson, John L. “When Lehi’s Party Arrived in the Land, Did They Find Others There?” Journal of Book of Mormon Studies 1/1 (1992): 1–34.
- Stubbs, Brian D. “The Labial Labyrinth in Uto-Aztecan.” The International Journal of American Linguistics 61/4 (1995): 394–420.
- Stubbs, Brian D. “A Comparative Vocabulary of Uto-Aztecan Languages.” In preparation.
- Stubbs, Silvia Canelo. Native speaker of Spanish from Salta and Tucuman, Argentina. Personal Communication.
- Voegelin, C.F. “A Working Dictionary of Tübatulabal,” International Journal of American Linguistics 24 (1958): 221–28.
- Voegelin, C. F., and F. M. Voegelin. Hopi Domains: A Lexical Approach to the Problem of Selection. Baltimore: Waverly Press, 1957.
- Watkins, Laurel. “Comparative Kiowa-Tanoan Materials.” In preparation.
- Williams, Ronald J. Hebrew Syntax, 2nd ed. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, 1976.
- Zepeda, Ofelia. A Papago Grammar. Tucson: University of Arizona Press, 1983.
- Zigmond, Maurice L., Curtis G. Booth, and Pamela Munro. Kawaiisu: A Grammar and Dictionary with Texts. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1991.
Notes
- See particularly John L. Sorenson, An Ancient American Setting for the Book of Mormon (Salt Lake City: Deseret Book and FARMS, 1985); and John L. Sorenson, “When Lehi’s Party Arrived in the Land, Did They Find Others There?” Journal of Book of Mormon Studies 1/1 (1992): 1–34. These two sources delineate evidence suggesting much-less-than-hemispheric limits to Book of Mormon geography and populations.
- This opinion is based upon the following experience: two years on a Navajo-speaking mission; five years of Hebrew; three years of Spanish; three years of Arabic; two years of German; one year each of Ancient Egyptian, Aramaic, and Sanskrit. Beyond languages backed by college credit, I am also presently compiling the largest Tewa dictionary in existence and a dictionary on the White Mesa Ute dialect. I have also studied to varying degrees Tarahumara, Hopi, Papago, Nahuatl, Quechua, Choctaw, and Samoan, and I have engaged in brief perusals of dozens of other languages. As a Uto-Aztecanist, I have published “The Labial Labyrinth in Uto-Aztecan,” in The International Journal of American Linguistics 61/4 (1995): 394-420; “The Comparative Value of Tubar in Uto-Aztecan” is scheduled to be published in a memorial volume for Professor Wick Miller; I have a third article “The Elusive Liquids of Uto-Aztecan” in preparation for IJAL; and I am presently completing a book entitled A Comparative Vocabulary of Uto-Aztecan Languages, which will be the largest work on comparative UA linguistics, adding comment and cognate sets to all noticed thus far in the literature; and I have started another book entitled The Language Puzzle of the Ancient Pueblo or Anasazi.
- Sorenson, “When Lehi’s Party Arrived in the Land, Did They Find Others There?” 1–34.
- Among Latter-day Saint scholars are a few Semitists, to whom queries regarding the validity of the Semitic data can be directed. As for Latter-day Saint Uto-Aztecanists, I know of no others besides myself. Therefore, because it may be difficult for nonspecialists to assess the merit of proposed linguistic connections, it may be well to mention that I have privately shared this material with five Uto-Aztecanists (linguists who have studied and published in UA linguistics) and four of the five were quite overwhelmed at the quantity and quality of the evidence—two spoke very highly of it; two, in surprise, could hardly speak at all after seeing it; and the fifth did not like the proposal generally, but offered no substantive refutations. For publications in Uto-Aztecan linguistics, see n. 2.
- *-ima/*-ima: For NW Sem *-ima, see Sabatino Moscati, ed., An Introduction to the Comparative Study of the Semitic Languages (Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz, 1964), 88 and 97, and John Huehnergard, Ugaritic Vocabulary in Syllabic Transcription, ed. Frank Moore Cross (Atlanta: Scholars Press, 1987), 296. For UA, the plural suffixes in a representative sample of UA languages are as follows:
Cp -im Hp -m N -me < *-ma Ca -em Sr -m Hch -ma Yq and My -im Tbr -m K -mï Wr -ima - All UA languages having this suffix show m; some show a vowel after the m (a, e, ï); and some show a high front vowel (i, e) before the m. Yq and My have –m suffixed to words ending in a vowel and –im suffixed to words ending in a consonant. UA languages tend toward CVCV patterns; thus, two adjacent vowels usually level to something between the two or the second often is eliminated, which process would explain the reduction of –im to –m after vowels. Something similar probably happened in the other UA languages that have no vowel before m, leaving –m or –mV in most UA languages. However, the presence of a high front vowel in at least four UA languages is a reality to be reckoned with that Uto-Aztecanists have ignored. If the vowel before m were excrescent in some way, a round vowel (o, u) would be more likely, but not i or e. The presence of a high front vowel before m strongly suggests an original high front vowel before m that was lost in the other languages. A reconstruction of *-ima seems most plausible since all variations from that can be attributed to vowel leveling—final a lowering i to e in Ca; and preceding i raising a to e or ï in some languages. As for N, Karen Dakin, “Phonological Changes in Nahuatl: The Tense, Aspect, Mood Systems,” International Journal of American Linguistics 45/1 (1979): 48–71, demonstrated that N –me came from an earlier *-ma. Wr has pairs like the following (morpheme divisions are Wick Miller’s in “Guarijio: Gramatica, Textos y Vocabulario,” 1989):
sg. su)ka-ni pl. su)ki-ma to sew sg. neha-ni pl. nehi-ma to hand over sg. ola-ni pl. ori-ma to shell corn - A morpheme division that includes the preceding vowel (which seems at least as reasonable) would yield sg. –ani and pl. –ima.
- *na-: Joshua Blau, A Grammar of Biblical Hebrew (Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz, 1976), perhaps the foremost Hebrew linguist-grammarian, renders the earlier voweling of the niqtal prefix as na– rather than ni-. He also lists examples that illustrate all three uses of the na– prefix: reflexive, reciprocal, and passive (ibid., 51). Though reflexive and reciprocal are the most common uses of the prefix in UA and passive is the most common use in biblical Hebrew, all three meanings are employed in Semitic and two of the three in UA. The semantic notions of reflexive, reciprocal, and passive often overlap in languages; for example, Spanish se is employed for all three uses, and in English the same event could be described with either “he burned himself” (reflexive) or “he got burned” (passive). Illustrations of the na- prefix in three UA languages are as follows:
SP paqï vt. bathe na-vaqï bathe oneself SP wï-ton)noi vt. shake na-nwï-ton)noi shake oneself Hp )öqala vt. greet s.o. naa-)öqala cheer oneself up Hp wïïsi brush, broom naa-wïïsi comb one’s hair Hp qöy-ta to start a fire naa-qöy-na burn oneself Tr co- vt. hit with the fist na-co- fight with each other Tr paba- vt. throw rocks at na-paba- throw rocks at each other - *yašiba/*yasipa: Verbs of temporary state in Semitic (such as *yašiba “sit”) generally exhibited i as the medial vowel of the perfect (Moscati, Comparative Study of the Semitic Languages, 122). However, the medial i later changed to a in most Hebrew verbs because of the closed stressed syllable created by the perfect suffixes (Blau, Grammar of Biblical Hebrew, 36; William Gesenius, Gesenius’ Hebrew Grammar, ed. E. Kautzsch and trans. A. E. Cowley, 2nd ed. [London: Oxford University Press, 1910], 120). Medial i is still apparent in the Aramaic form yətib and Ugaritic )atib. In addition, the short final vowels of proto-Semitic were lost in Hebrew (Moscati, Comparative Study of the Semitic Languages, 122, 170; Blau, Grammar of Biblical Hebrew, 30). Thus, UA showing *yasipa in light of pre-Hebrew *yašiba, even though classical Hebrew has yašab, is rather astounding. The UA forms are as follows:
Hopi yesiva Tr )asiba Od dahiva ST daivo Yq yesa - Od and ST, as members of the Tepiman branch of UA, have d corresponding to UA y, and h corresponding to UA s. So they also point to UA *yasipa. For the b and v elements, Uto-Aztecanists reconstruct *p, though b and v are exactly the allophonic variants of Hebrew/Semitic b (Hebrew yašab < *yašiba). Some Uto-Aztecanists consider the final -pa element to be a fossilized suffix of some sort, since Hp yesi and Tr )asi and Od dahi are also verb forms of those verbs in those languages.
- See *yašiba in n. 5.
- Wick R. Miller, Uto-Aztecan Cognate Sets (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1967), abbreviated as (UACS). UACS #262 lightning: My berok-tiria; Yq and My berok/be)ok; Andres Lionnet, Los Elementos de la Lengua Cahita (Mexico City: Universidad Nacional Autonona de Mexico, 1977); NT vïpïdoxudami; ST vïpgi; Od vïpïgi; Od bebedki “thunder.” In these words Hebrew b appears to correspond to UA *p, as it usually does, except in initial position. However, considering that the vowels have assimilated to the consonants’ point of articulation (baraq > berok, raising and fronting before alveolar r, and raising and backing before uvular q), the NT -dox- and Yq/My -rok- syllables help show nicely the presence of all three consonants: a bilabial, r, and k/q. The two Od forms may be Tepiman dialect variants or borrowings within Tepiman. Nevertheless, Od bebedki “thunder” shows nicely all three consonants as expected for Sem brq, with a slight semantic change.
- SP kani “kidney” and Hp kele-vosna “kidney” suggest PUA *kali. That form is possible in Northwest Semitic. Aramaic has both kolya and kulya. The Hebrew form appears only in the pl. klayôt, with a presumed singular of kilya, though the sg. is unattested. Nevertheless, a number of UA forms show a where Masoretic Hebrew shows i.
- In light of Hebrew katep “shoulder” and Ar katip/katp “shoulder,” consider Od kotva/kotova “shoulder”; Wr tehpóba “back” and “shoulder”; and Tr na-)tapu “push with the shoulder.” Wr alone shows the Sem vowel, though it is missing the first consonant; however, Tr is nearly missing the first consonant, but shows the frequently occurring ) for k in clusters, which makes the Wr clearer since it is nearly identical to Tr—Tr/Wr *)tep/*)tap. Nevertheless, all three Semitic consonants are well represented in their expected forms: UA *k, *t, *p.
- Hebrew šekem/šikm “shoulder”: Pn sikkum-pï “shoulder blade”; Sh sikkum-pï “shoulder blade”; Mn sihkuhpi “shoulder blade”; WM Ute sku-pï “shoulder”; Sr suka “shoulder”; Ls soka “shoulder”; Ca and Cp sek)a “shoulder”; Tr and Wr seka “arm, hand”; NT ïka “arm”; My koxm-im “arm(s)”; Yq komim “arm”; Hp sïkapci “scapula of sheep”; Hp sïkakci “shoulder blade.”
- Hebrew )adam “man”; NT odami “person”; Od o)odham “person, tribesman, man”; ST odam “man”; Yq and My )o)ow “man, person,” pl. )o)ow-im; Tbr oñwi “man”; Tr owí “male, macho”; Wr oí “male, macho.”
- The rounding effect of the )aleph or glottal stop causes the initial vowel to be )o (cf. 52–64). The Tepiman languages (NT and Od) preserve all else fairly well. Yq and My often have r and ) alternations (cf. berok/be)ok “lightning”) and with intervocalic d easily being perceived as intervocalic r (as it is in English), it only remains to explain m > w (odam > orom > o)ow). In Tbr the intervening vowel was lost to create an alveolar-nasal cluster (dm) in which the alveolar became a nasal (n), and the m a w, which was probably nasalized in this now extinct UA language; for *m becomes a nasalized w in Ute very often. From that Tr and Wr owí “male” were probably derived, whether by similar development as Tbr or by borrowing from Tbr.
- Cp meme-t “ocean” and Ls moma-t both fit a reconstruction of *meme-t “ocean,” since the Ls o does correspond to Cp e.
- Hebrew šapa(y) “sweep bare, smooth”; Gesenius gives “scrape off”; in later Hebrew qittel šippah “plane off”; Mn sipa “shave”; Cm sibe “scrape, shave”; Tb siip “shave”; Hp sipaw-ta/sispa “shave.” Not only does the i vowel in UA suggest a qittel form rather than qal, but p in Hp instead of v also suggests qittel with its doubled medial consonant; otherwise, intervocalic p in Hp allophonically becomes v. A note convenient at this point is that lamed-he verbs (those which end with h in Hebrew writing) will be represented rmy/ramah, even though the final h in written Hebrew is basically an orthographic device to demonstrate a final vowel sound. However, h is not the third consonant and never was pronounced unless it is mappiq with a dot in it. This is quite apparent in the Hebrew and Arabic forms of rmy: rama ____, ramiti ____, rama(y) ____, ramaytu ____, rumiyat ____. Nevertheless, even in English transcriptions that final h has become something of an orthographic institution among Semitists that we shall momentarily conform to.
- The Hebrew Old Testament constitutes the majority of ancient Hebrew texts. Because not all spoken vocabulary would have found its way into the ancient text(s), certain items in other Semitic languages found to correspond to UA are worth noting, since those items could well have been in the spoken Hebrew language regardless their lack in an ancient text. The word for “squirrel” is an example. There is no word for “squirrel” in the Old Testament text; it simply did not occur in the writings of the scribes and prophets. However, the Arabic word for “squirrel” sinjab would correspond to Hebrew siggôb (< *singab), and curiously we find UA sikku “squirrel,” exactly as expected with the typical raising of vowels, loss of final consonant, and even the geminated medial consonant.
- When n is the first element in a consonant cluster, Hebrew typically assimilates it to double the second consonant, whereas Arabic does not:
Ar )anfuhu Heb )appô his nose Ar bint Heb batt- daughter Ar hinta Heb hitta wheat - In addition, long a of Arabic and proto-Semitic correspond to o in Hebrew; therefore, an Arabic form of sinjab “squirrel” would yield siggôb in Hebrew. And SP sikku “squirrel” is exactly what we would expect with the usual rising of vowels in UA and loss of a final segment. Some might argue s or š (sh), but Arabic’s s (sin) can correspond to either Hebrew samech or shin; it hardly matters, however, since all three Semitic s‘s (s1, s2, s3) merge to UA s.
- When n is the first element in a consonant cluster, Hebrew typically assimilates it to double the second consonant, whereas Arabic does not:
- Hebrew sippôr “bird, small bird.” Tr cíburi “chicks, baby birds”; Od sipug “bird, cardinal”; Ca and Cp cíip (in compound words for birds); Wr cu)rukí “bird.” Od s does correspond to UA c; therefore, Od sipu < UA *cipu. The final g in Od is probably related to the final –kí syllable in Wr, both of which are probably another morpheme of an older compound.
- See n. 17 below.
- The correspondence of bilabials (b, p, w) and labio-velars (kw, gw) occurs often: in Indo-European (Greek p, Latin kw), Uto-Aztecan (*kw > b, bw, w, kw), Spanish dialects, etc. Where my wife, Silvia Canelo, grew up, the Spanish dialect had such pronunciations as gweno (< bueno “good”), gwevo (< huevo “egg”), and gweso (< hueso “bone”).
- In the phonology of the Masoretic dialect of Hebrew, Semitic b became spirant or fricative v when following vowels and not doubled. Its pronunciation remained the voiced bilabial stop when geminated (doubled) or in initial position or when following another consonant. Interesting in regard to UA is that Hebrew dageshed b‘s correspond to UA *kw, but non-dageshed b‘s correspond to UA *p, and thus merged with Sem p, which also corresponds to UA *p. However, a doubled pp often also corresponds to UA *kw, as does the doubled bb. The Wr form for bird in 13 above (Wr cu)rukí) is an example, since u) is a typical reflex of kw in a cluster (with r, in this case). The whole matter requires more investigation; nevertheless, it generally appears that the nearer a Hebrew allophone is to the upper left corner in the paradigm below, the greater the probability of a correspondence with UA *kw, and the nearer it is to the lower right corner, the more likely is a correspondence with UA *p (> p/v).
bb b v pp p f - A thorough treatment of the labial complexities from a strictly Uto-Aztecan point of view is treated in Stubbs, “The Labyrinth in Uto-Aztecan,” 374–420.
- In the phonology of the Masoretic dialect of Hebrew, Semitic b became spirant or fricative v when following vowels and not doubled. Its pronunciation remained the voiced bilabial stop when geminated (doubled) or in initial position or when following another consonant. Interesting in regard to UA is that Hebrew dageshed b‘s correspond to UA *kw, but non-dageshed b‘s correspond to UA *p, and thus merged with Sem p, which also corresponds to UA *p. However, a doubled pp often also corresponds to UA *kw, as does the doubled bb. The Wr form for bird in 13 above (Wr cu)rukí) is an example, since u) is a typical reflex of kw in a cluster (with r, in this case). The whole matter requires more investigation; nevertheless, it generally appears that the nearer a Hebrew allophone is to the upper left corner in the paradigm below, the greater the probability of a correspondence with UA *kw, and the nearer it is to the lower right corner, the more likely is a correspondence with UA *p (> p/v).
- A clear correspondence of r to y exists in the Mayan language family. Lyle Campbell, Quichean Linguistic Prehistory (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1977), 97–100. A less clear correspondence of r to y/i exists in Athapaskan. Harry Hoijer, Studies in Athapaskan Languages (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1963), 19:
Ingalik: sruš bear sran summer zrun black Kutchin: syí bear syín summer zrei black Navajo: šaš bear ši summer zin black - English creoles show similar phenomena: for > fo, fi, foe. Derek Bickerton, Roots of Language (Ann Arbor: Karoma, 1981), 61. So to find an r to y/i correspondence in the Hebrew-UA connection is not so unusual; nevertheless, though y is the reflex in most of UA, UA *y corresponds to Tepiman d, and r itself appears on occasion in some of the Sonoran languages.
- Arabic dabb “lizard” and dabba “take hold of, keep under lock, to bolt”; Hebrew sab “lizard” (< *sabb). (Keep in mind Ar d = Hebrew s.) This is an unusual semantic pair from the same root, which I assume to be understood in the lizard’s grasp being perceived like a lock. Nevertheless, regardless of the semantic connection, UA has the same unusual pair of meanings as Semitic: Ca caxwa–l (< *cakwa) “lizard” and N cakwa “to enclose, lock up.” Consider also Ls cakwi hold, catch; Cp cakwe “grab, cling to”; Eu capa– “grab,” and Od šaku “hold in the palm,” for Od š = UA c.
- Hebrew basar “flesh” has a secondary meaning of penis (Ezekiel 16:26; 23:20). In UA it means “tail” in most languages, “penis” in Hp, and “flesh” in NT. Interestingly, Coptic sat/set means both “tail” and “penis,” a Near Eastern language with a similar semantic combination as is found in UA. Thomas O. Lambdin, Introduction to Sahidic Coptic (Macon, GA: Mercer University Press, 1983), 266.
- Hebrew šabber (qittel impf stem) “to break, break in pieces”; Hp sakwi-ta “break off, tear down, ruin”; Ca sakway “to mess up” SP cukkwi “crush.”
- Hebrew dabber (qittel impf) “to speak, talk.” Mn tïïhkwii “tell, say”; SP tïkwinna to tell a story. Also of interest, from the Sem root is a noun Hebrew dabar “word, thing.” Consider Tr tabiri “thing,” and N tepi “small thing.” Note also the UA *kw correspondence for Hebrew doubled bb, and the UA *p correspondence for intervocalic nongeminated Hebrew b (cf. n. 18 above).
- Sem krr/krkr (a derived form of krr; see BDB 502) “go in circles, dance.” SP kiya “to have a round dance.”
- Ar mrr/marra “go, travel.” UA *miya “go”; Mn miya; Sh mia; Ute miya; Sr mi, miaaTo); Tb miy; Od med (remember Od d < UA *y).
- The three diverse semantic dimensions of Semitic brr are the verbal meaning “select, choose”; the noun Hebrew bar “field,” Ar barr “land”; and Hebrew bar “grain.” UA has three similar sets of meanings: the verbal meaning in N kwi “take”; the meaning of land in UA *kwiya “land, earth, dirt” in Ls, Od, Tr, Wr, My, Tbr, Cr; and a grain in UA *kwi/kwiya “acorn” in SP, Ute, Cp, Ls, Gb, Sr, Hp. In some of those languages, the kwi is combined with other suffixed morphemes.
- Aramaic and late Hebrew šrq/šaraq “to comb, card”; UA *siyuk “to comb”; Tb siuk “to comb”; Ute ciyu)wey “to comb”; perhaps Ca suyavis “comb,” n.
- Hebrew bsr/basar “to enclose, cut off, make inaccessible.” Ute kwocayai “to wrap around”; Od biiš “to corner.” Od b corresponds to UA *kw; and Od s/š to UA *c. So the consonants all correspond perfectly, though the vowels have other possible explanations.
- Hebrew bô “in it” actually consists of two parts: the consonant b “in” can be prefixed to any noun or pronoun meaning “in something”; the –o is a suffix for third person singular masculine nouns.
- See n. 17.
- Hebrew gebîm “locust” (< *gebim). SP qï:vi “locust” (< *kïpi).
- Ar daniy “low.” N nani “below”; and perhaps UACS #35 *tena “below.”
- Hebrew dayeq “siege-wall.” Hp tïyïqa “wall.”
- Hebrew deqel “palm tree”; Ar daqal “palm tree.” UA *taku “palm tree” in My, Tr, Wr, Eu, Tbr, and Hch. We would normally expect ï instead of u, but the two are close; either a Spanish-speaking ear not hearing the distinction or Spanish influence changing ï to u could explain it.
- Hebrew dqr/daqar “pierce.” UA *tekiy “cut”; N teki “cut”; Hp tïkï “cut”; Od cekid “vaccinate, drive a stake”; Ca ceki/ciki “stick in.” Od shows the third consonant, since Od d corresponds to UA *y and Hebrew r, as well as Od c corresponding to UA t before high vowels.
- Hebrew hes and hesî “arrow.” UACS #9 arrow: SP uu; Hp ho-hï; NT ui; Od )uš arrowhead; Sr hoc. Od š and Sr c both equate to final c; Hp o = PUA *u; and all these forms plus others show initial hu/u for the pharyngeal h; thus, all add up quite nicely to UA *huc(i), exactly as expected for Sem hes(i), since in hypothetical *hueci, the second vowel of a diphthong seldom survives.
- Hebrew hrk “set in motion”; Ar haruka “move.” UACS #296 move: Tb )oyog-at/)oyok “be moving”; Hp hoyo (sg.), hoyok-ya (pl.) “move.”
- Hebrew hpp “rub, cleanse.” Tr úba “bathe”; Wr u)upá “bathe”; Eu úva– “bathe”; Yq )uba “bathe”; My )ubba “bathe”; Hch )ïïva “bathe.” Hch ï = PUA *u.
- Hebrew hmr “cover or smear” (with asphalt). Ca humay “smear, paint.”
- Hebrew hll “to play the pipe.” Tb lulu)/)ululu) “play the flute,” and others.
- Ar )hh/)ahaha “to cough.” UACS #105 to cough: Hp )öhöhö-ta; Ca )u)uhu; Tb hoh-/)ohoh; and others.
- Hebrew srh “cry, roar.” Tb cayau “yell.”
- Hebrew impf yismah “sprout” (of trees, grass); N icmo-lini “sprout, grow.” Nouns in various UA languages meaning “grass” also fit, but require more explanation.
- Hebrew slh “rush.” N coloa “flee, run swiftly.”
- Hebrew s(q/sa(aq “cry, cry out, call.” UACS #114 *coak “to cry.”
- Hebrew šg( “be mad.” N sikoa “feel envy, suffer.”
- Hebrew š(( “delight in.” N na-soa “love, value, cherish.”
- Hebrew bl( “swallow. ” Hp kwelo “to taste”; Tb weleh “swallow” (UA *kw = Tb w).
- Hebrew rega( “(in a) moment.” Tr rekó “soon, in a short time.”
- Hebrew se(ar “hair”; Ar ša(r/ša(ar “hair”; ša(ira “be hairy.” UA *suwi “hair” in several languages.
- Hebrew na(ar “boy, young man”; na(ara “girl.” Tr nowi “have a son”; Tr no “son”; Wr nu)íti/nu)ínti “child”; UACS #472a *nawi “girl”: Pn nawiccibi; Tb )anawiš-t; Ls nawíi-l; Ca náwiš-mal.
- Hebrew ya(ar “forest, wood.” Ca yuyi-l “California Juniper”; Ca yuyivaš “pines with long needles”; SP yïvi “long-needled pine” (w > v in SP); Hp yo-völö “chipmunk” (< tree-innards).
- Hebrew d(k “to go out” (of fire). UA *tuk “to go out” (of fire): SP tukwa; Ca tuq; Od cuuk. Also UA *tuk “become dark, night.” “Black” in several languages.
- Hebrew ps( “to bruise.” N pacoa “to bruise”; N paciwi “be bruised.”
- Hebrew (ly/(alah “to go up, ascend, climb.” Ca wel “rise up high, grow”;Tb )ool– “get up, fly”; Hp )o-miq “up-toward” (-miq = toward); N wal “come/hither”; however, in Nahuatl compounds the general meaning of “go up, increase” is left after subtracting the meaning of the other compounded element:
N walkisa “come up” (of sun, or out of water) (N kisa come out); N walweci “to fall from a high place” (N weci fall); N wallalia “to augment, increase” (N nalia “to be placed, situated”)
;Consider also, in connection with the hiqtîl meaning of “cause to go up in smoke, sacrifice” (participle ma(aleh), Wr molo “to make smoke.” Consider also Hebrew ma(al “upward, above” and Tr mo “up, upward.”
- Hebrew ma(alah “steps, stairs, ascent.” Wr i)mola “stairs”; Wr i)mola-ni “to have an ascent or climb” (of a road, path).
- Ar (agaza “to grow old” (of women); (aguz “old woman, old man.” Tr wegaca “to grow old” (of women); Od oks “old woman.” The Semitic and Tr verbs not only match phonologically and semantically in “grow old,” but specifically “of women.” The Od form may also be a likely match in that Od s corresponds to UA c; thus, outside of a vowel reduction between the two consonants, Od as well as some of the following may be connected with this root also: UACS #473 *)ok “woman”: NT oks; Cr )uka-ri “old woman”; Hch )uka. Perhaps also N okic-ni “man,” if originally “old man.”
- Ar d(w/da(a “to call, summon, name.” UACS #300 *tewa “name”; this common UA word has either a nominal or verbal reflex in a number of UA languages.
- Hebrew )arî “lion.” Wr wori “mountain lion”; cognate forms also in Tbr, Yq, and My.
- Hebrew )îš “man”; with negatives “no one.” Tr wesi (<*wïsi) “someone”: with negatives “no one.”
- Hebrew )ešet/)išt- “woman, wife.” Hp wïïti “wife.” All quite as expected, if from the possessed form )išt-, since š in clusters disappears but often leaves its trace in the vowel ï. Perhaps SP wicci “great grandmother.”
- Hebrew g)l/ga)al “redeem, pay for.” N kowa “buy”; Ca )u)uwe “buy.”
- Hebrew and Ar qr) “call, cry.” N te-koyoa “howl”; N koyo-n?”coyote.”
- Hebrew pe)ah “corner, sideburn.” UA *powa/po)a “hair” in several languages.
- Ar *pa)r “mouse.” Mn puwe-; SP pu)i– ; Ute pu)úy-; Sr pa)iš; Hp pöhsa all meaning “mouse.”
- The term tirmania “truffle” is probably not of Semitic origin, but it is a Mediterranean term for a kind of truffle, whatever its origin. Charles Heimsch, The Encyclopaedia Americana (New York: Americana Corporation, 1962), s.v. “truffle.”
- N cawa “spin”; Od šo(m) “sew a seam on.”
- Hebrew hût “thread, cord”; Ar xayt “thread, twine.” UACS #419 string: Sr wicï)-t; Mn wihsi; My witeri, wi)i; Hch wiita; Wr wohci “cord.” The expected reflex for Sem t would be UA c; UA shows t, c, and s, of which c is somewhat a phonological mean; as well, UA within itself has many c/s or c/t alternations. Many Semitic roots of medial semivowel can show both w or y; the UA forms agree with y.
- For Od ušabi “resin, pitch,” the š of Od corresponds to UA c, so all is as expected, though most non-dageshed Hebrew b‘s would be p/v in Od rather than b (=UA *kw).
- Hebrew matteh “staff, rod, branch.” Hp komaci “firewood” (*ku/ko = fire).
- Two closely related roots, Hebrew rbb/rabab/robb “shoot” and Hebrew rby/raba “shoot” compare with Ute tonkwa “snap” (of bow) for the doubled b and Ch tavi “hit, stone s.th.” for the second form, as well as perhaps Hp tïïva “throw” and several other UA languages.
- Hebrew r(m “to thunder”; Hebrew ra(am “thunder,” n. Sh tompai “thunder”; Ca tawva-l “thunder”; Od toahim “thunder”; words for “thunder,” “cloud,” and “winter” seem to overlap in UA. (Hp L < *w) UACS #93 *tom “cloud”: Mn to “cloud”; Mn to-yaqa “thunder”; Cm tomoa– “cloud”; Ls toma-wut “thunder”; ST tuva); Wr tomuari “cloud.” Miller also compares these forms with UACS #467 *tomo “winter” (several languages).
- Ar rbt/rabata/-rbit “to tie, bind.” UACS #438 *tapi/tapic “to tie”: SP tahpica-: Cr ratapi)iste; Hch –tapi “knot, tie a knot.”
- Hebrew rš( “be wicked, guilty.” Tr rasewa “fornicate”; Tr rasewa-me “permissive person”; Tb tïsawin “cause s.o. evil”; Tb tïšï “be bad”; SP –rïssu)ai–na)ai “not heeding, paying no attention”; perhaps Tr risiwa/risoa “pain, suffering, hardship.”
- Ar rajul “man” (<*ragul). Tr rehoy “man”; Wr tihoé “man”; Od ce)oj “man”; Kiowa togul “young man.” The three UA forms (Tr, Wr, Od) point to *tïhoy, suggestive of Sem ragul, with a change of g > h, and l > y/i/e like r (which happens often but is not treated in this brief summary). The second consonant’s sound change could use stronger support; the first and third, however, are common and consistent. Most intriguing and supportive for UA *tïhoy “man” is Kiowa togul “young man,” which shows perfectly all three consonants, including the g and l (initial *r > t; *g = g; *l = l), with the first vowel assimilating to the second.
- Hebrew roš “head”; Ar ra)s– “head.” SP tocci “head”; Ch toc(i) “head.” A short paper does not allow treatment of all matters; nevertheless, items 16, 21, and 54 are additional examples that show the tendency of Semitic š corresponding to Numic c.
- Hebrew kanap “wing.” UACS #465 *)ana “wing” (also “arm” and “feather”): Tr )ana; Hch )ana; SP anapu-/anapï; Tb )anambïï-l; Od )a)an; and others. SP and Tb show the third consonant p, the others only the first and second.
- Hebrew kinnîm “gnats.” UACS #288 mosquito: SP )ani-; Mn )anipi; Cm )animui; Cr huna.
- Hebrew geled/gild– “skin.” Od )eldag “skin” (of person); Od )eldaj “hide” (of animal).
- Hebrew gll/galal “to roll”; gullah “basin, bowl” (from round shape); gel/galal (ball of) “dung.” Hp nölö “loop, circle, coil”; Hp nöla “hair-whorl, tire”; Hp nölöla “bend”; Od )ola “ball, sphere,” and other UA forms.
- Hebrew qereb = “inside.” Tepiman languages show *)era/*erap “in.”
- Ronald W. Langacker, An Overview of Uto-Aztecan Grammar, vol. 1, Studies in Uto-Aztecan Grammar (Arlington: The University of Texas at Arlington and Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1977), 124, 126.
- Hugh W. Nibley, “Lehi and the Arabs,” in An Approach to the Book of Mormon, 3rd ed. (Salt Lake City: Deseret Book and FARMS, 1988), 71–83.
- UA *taka “man” in several languages.
- Hebrew z”)eb “wolf”; Ar #948;i)b “wolf.” SP tïva “wolf”; Tb tïbaic “wolf”; Cr ï:ra)abe “wolf”; Hch ïrawe “wolf”; and perhaps Od šee)e “wolf,” though Od should show c rather than š for UA t.
- Hebrew zaqan “beard, chin”; likewise Ar #948;aqan and Akk ziqnu. Several UA languages show *ten(i) “mouth”; however, again Tr shows the crucial glottal stop as a vestige of the lost uvular in a cluster: Tr re)na.
- See nn. 74 and 82.
- Another etymology has been suggested for Pima; as Dunnigan puts it, “The most frequently cited folk etymology for the origin of the word Pima is that it is a corruption of the O’odham expression pi )an)maat, literally ‘I don’t know.’ Supposedly, this was the native’s answer to the first interrogations in Spanish”; quoted from Timothy Dunnigan, “Lower Pima,” in Handbook of American Indians, vol. 10, ed. Alfonso Ortiz (Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution, 1983), 229.
- Sorenson, An Ancient American Setting.
- I heard Wick Miller cite this view, whether his own opinion or in conference with other Uto-Aztecanists, I am not sure. Nor am I sure it matters, since Wick Miller was probably the foremost Uto-Aztecanist until his recent untimely death.
- Adobe is a borrowing into English from Spanish, though ultimately from Arabic, Coptic, and Egyptian probably; nevertheless, its first occurrence in print in English is 1834, after the Book of Mormon’s publication, and it did not become a commonly used word in English until several decades after Joseph Smith’s time. OED 1:123.
- Wilford Woodruff in a letter to John Taylor and Council, dated 15 September 1879, expressed this view. “Nephites Found in New Mexico,” in A Book of Mormon Treasury (Salt Lake City: Bookcraft, 1959), 222–27.
Subject Keywords
Bibliographic Citation
Terms of use
Items in the BMC Archive are made publicly available for non-commercial, private use. Inclusion within the BMC Archive does not imply endorsement. Items do not represent the official views of The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-day Saints or of Book of Mormon Central.